Friday, June 20, 2014
Gordon Conference: Unifying Ecology Across Scales
The Gordon Conference on Unifying Ecology Across Scales is open for registration. It runs July 20-24, and should be an interesting meeting and well worth going.
Tuesday, June 10, 2014
Valuing Toronto's urban forest: seeing the forest for the trees
Many news outlets in Toronto reported on a study
released by the chief economist at TD bank about the value of urban trees in
Toronto. Toronto has been called ‘the city in a park’ because of the heavily
forested urban landscape we have here (though when you Google ‘city in a park’
a plethora of cities have the same view of themselves). The value of Toronto’s
urban forest estimated by the economists was 7 billion dollars. This seems like
an astronomical amount, and that a large bank is the one forwarding this view
of the value of an urban forest is in itself an amazing development (note: I do have an
obvious conflict of interest as my professorship is endowed by TD).
TD's valuation of the urban forest relied on per-species
estimates of net benefits, including carbon sequestration, air quality
improvement, storm water flow, and energy savings though shading of buildings.
These economic returns more than justify municipal expenses for maintaining
parks and urban trees. This approach to quantifying the value of trees has been
forwarded by new initiatives such as iTree that provide information on the
benefits of tree species. The TD report does go on to say that there are other
unquantified benefits of the urban forest such as aesthetic values and
importance to communities. But the question is, is cumulative economic benefit
a sum of individual trees or is there something more to a forest?
While individual trees have clear economic benefits,
captured nicely in the report, and which often increase with the age or size of
the tree, there may be direct economic benefit from forested lands that is
greater than the sum of the individual trees. In essence, we need to see the
additional value of the forest for the trees. Individual trees do not make a
forest, and there is something special about a forest.
The simplest way in which a forest supplies additional value
is through diversity effects. Different tree species may utilize differing resources or niches and by occurring together are able to
turn more of the total local resources into growth, thus sequestering more
carbon dioxide then if they were grown alone or only with other trees of the
same species. As an example, if you grow a tall canopy tree and a medium shade
tolerant species underneath it, the cumulative energy savings through shading
are much greater than growing two tall canopy trees or two medium shade tolerant
trees. This is often referred to as ‘complementarity’
More than species complementing one another, in forests we
often see species facilitating one another, meaning that individual trees
perform better with other tree species around it, then when grown alone. Again,
using carbon sequestration as the example, facilitation means that more carbon
is taken up then when trees are isolated from one another.
Forests also provide habit for other plants and animals that
individual trees do not provide. A forest can also better support pollinators
by including different tree species that flower at different times of the
season. Further, forests provide recreational activities (biking, hiking,
camping, etc.) that are economically measurable (gas costs to travel there,
user fees, tax support, etc.).
Taken all together, intact forests supply even greater
economic, health, and environmental benefits than individual trees. If the
trees of Toronto are valued at 7 billion dollars, then the forests of Toronto
must be worth much more.
Tuesday, June 3, 2014
Monday, May 19, 2014
Guest Post: You teach science, but is your teaching scientific? Part 2: Flipping your class.
The second in a series of guest posts about using scientific teaching, active learning, and flipping the classroom by Sarah Seiter, a teaching fellow at the University of Colorado, Boulder.
When universities first opened in the middle ages, lecturing was the most cutting edge information technology available to a professor - books were copied by hand so the fastest way to transfer information was to talk at your students (see the awesome TED talk below for a breakdown of how universities can and should change). Lecturing is still the default at most universities, and faculty spend hours developing their lecture skills. But studies have shown over and over again that lecturing is one of the worst possible ways to get students to learn. This means that our most accomplished scientists are working like crazy to master a method of teaching that is straight up medieval.
Lecturing isn’t going away any time soon, but you do a lot for your classes by incorporating active learning techniques, sometimes called “flipping” a class. The main feature of a flipped class is that students do the knowledge acquisition (the lecture-like) part of the course at home, and then do “homework” in the classroom with the instructor and peers to help them apply knowledge.
Flipping your classroom will probably not result in chaos. Nobody is going to smash the conch shell and kill Piggy, but they might learn something.
DIY Case Studies: If you have papers that you already like to teach, then consider turning them into a case study. To do this, I usually write an intro briefly framing the problem or question. Then I give students actual graphs from the paper with follow up questions to help them process the information. It is OK if the study has a few confusing elements; while we often want a clear story to present to our classes, there’s great evidence that using “messy” data builds scientific skills. You may have to modify graphs, or remake them for extra readability. This could mean re-labeling axes to remove jargon (e.g. in a paper on insects, “instar” becomes “developmental stage”). It might mean dropping some treatments (you don’t need 10 nitrogen treatments to understand eutrophication). Usually I follow every graph with 2-3 questions that follow a basic format:
Using Case Studies In Class: You can prepare students for a case either through a short lecture or through a homework assignment or reading quiz (this can be done using classroom management software like Blackboard, or Sakai). Once students have the background, have them break into groups of two to three, and work through the questions. It can be helpful to stop every few minutes to go through the answers (some case studies build on earlier questions, so early feedback is key). A great feature of case studies is that they can take nearly an entire class period, so you can go an entire day without having to lecture.
Writing Good Clicker Questions: Good clicker questions should encourage discussion, and force students to apply their knowledge, not just test what they remember. This can mean using information to make recommendations, doing a calculation, or making predictions about the outcome of experiment. Standard clickers only allow for multiple choice questions, but other web-based tools will allow your students to do free responses, draw graphs, or give other types of answers. There are lots of great web resources on how to design clicker questions (in appendix). The slide show below shows some clicker questions we used in our flipped evolution class at CU Boulder.
Using Clickers In Class: Once you have your clicker questions written, then you’ll need to deploy them in class. Below is a basic blueprint for how to run a clicker question
1. Tell students to break into groups and get ready to discuss a clicker question
2. Give students about a minute to discuss the question, and open whatever clicker software you’re using. You’ll usually hear a 30 second surge in talking that dies down after about a minute. After about a minute give students a warning and tell then close out the clicker question.
3. At this point you can show the results of the clicker poll and start to unpack the question. If your questions are challenging, you should be getting significant amounts of wrong answers, so seeing a wide range of answers means you’re doing it right. Usually if 10% of your students are getting the question wrong, it is worth discussing the question in depth
4. Make students be able to articulate why right answers are right and why wrong answers are wrong. You can call on groups to get them to explain their answers (this is nicer than cold-calling individual students). If nobody wants to talk about wrong answers, say something like “why might someone think that B is a tempting answer?” so that nobody has to admit to being wrong in front of their peers.
5. It can be helpful to follow up with another question asking them to apply the material in a different way.
In conclusion, flipping your classroom can be done pretty cheaply and without that much more work than lecturing. This post is really just a starting place, and there are ton of great resources on the web to take you further. I’ve compiled just a few of them below. Good luck and happy flipping!
By Sarah Seiter
Videos on Flipped Classrooms:
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=EMhJcwvmamY
Resources for Clicker Qs:
Clicker Question Guides from University of Colorado Boulder
http://www.colorado.edu/sei/documents/clickeruse_guide0108.pdf
http://www.slideshare.net/stephaniechasteen/writing-great-clicker-questions
Vanderbilt:
http://cft.vanderbilt.edu/guides-sub-pages/clickers/
Flipped Classroom Fears:
Instructors often imagine a Lord of The Flies style scenario when they start flipping their classrooms, but this isn’t usually the case. In fact, most students are actually so conditioned to sit quietly in class that it can be difficult to get them to talk about the material. However, there are a few things you can do to get students in the frame of mind for productive discussion.Flipping your classroom will probably not result in chaos. Nobody is going to smash the conch shell and kill Piggy, but they might learn something.
- Start small: If you’re just getting into transforming your class, it can be helpful to start with something small, like flipping once a week.
- Get extra staff: Since group work key to flipped classroom, it helps to have extra staff to facilitate peer discussions. If you have graduate TAs, consider deputizing them to lead group exercises. If your university has an undergrad TA program, get as many as you can and spend a day training them on how to ask good questions and facilitate conversations.
- Explain to students why flipping works: Students will sometimes complain if they’re used to sitting passively in lecture, and they’re suddenly forced to do homework in class. But flipping builds skills that they’ll need in the workplace or graduate school, so reemphasizing what they’re gaining can help get them to buy in.
Tools For Flipping: Case Studies
Case studies usually involve taking scientific data or ideas and then applying them to a real world situation (medical, law and business schools have been using them for years). Case studies are all over the internet, although the largest clearing house is the National Case Study Library (the American Museum of Natural History, the National Geographic Society, the Smithsonian, and the Understanding Evolution project at Berkeley also have great resources). The National Case Study Library is the largest and is searchable by topic and age, and includes teaching notes for each case, and can be a great place to get started.
Picking Case Studies: Some case studies are purely hypothetical, but I tend to gravitate to those that use real data from published studies like this one on the evolution of skin color that uses studies from a lot of disciplines to build to a conclusion, or this one on conservation corridors and meta-populations. A lot of case studies open with a fictional story, but this approach is a little corny for me, and I’d rather focus on the real scientists and their questions (the narrative case studies can also get weird (like this paternity case study that could also be a great Maury episode). In general, just pick things that work for you and your students.
DIY Case Studies: If you have papers that you already like to teach, then consider turning them into a case study. To do this, I usually write an intro briefly framing the problem or question. Then I give students actual graphs from the paper with follow up questions to help them process the information. It is OK if the study has a few confusing elements; while we often want a clear story to present to our classes, there’s great evidence that using “messy” data builds scientific skills. You may have to modify graphs, or remake them for extra readability. This could mean re-labeling axes to remove jargon (e.g. in a paper on insects, “instar” becomes “developmental stage”). It might mean dropping some treatments (you don’t need 10 nitrogen treatments to understand eutrophication). Usually I follow every graph with 2-3 questions that follow a basic format:
- Question 1: Ask students to detect any trends or differences in the graph.
- Question 2: Have students think of an explanation for the results
- Question 3: Ask students to apply their “findings” to the question or problem posed in the case study
Using Case Studies In Class: You can prepare students for a case either through a short lecture or through a homework assignment or reading quiz (this can be done using classroom management software like Blackboard, or Sakai). Once students have the background, have them break into groups of two to three, and work through the questions. It can be helpful to stop every few minutes to go through the answers (some case studies build on earlier questions, so early feedback is key). A great feature of case studies is that they can take nearly an entire class period, so you can go an entire day without having to lecture.
Clicker Questions
The other main tool for flipping your classroom is clicker questions. Clickers are basically a real-time poll of your students so you can check how they are learning. Most instructors use them for participation points, rather than grading them for correctness (this encourages students to jump in and grapple with material, and not worry about making mistakes). Your university might have a set of clickers that you can borrow, or you have students use laptops, tablets and smartphones in place of clicker with apps like Poll Everywhere, GoSoapbox, Pinnion, or Socrative (these have different features and price points, so see what works for you). For a more comprehensive list of clicker tools , see this article from a team at Princeton.Writing Good Clicker Questions: Good clicker questions should encourage discussion, and force students to apply their knowledge, not just test what they remember. This can mean using information to make recommendations, doing a calculation, or making predictions about the outcome of experiment. Standard clickers only allow for multiple choice questions, but other web-based tools will allow your students to do free responses, draw graphs, or give other types of answers. There are lots of great web resources on how to design clicker questions (in appendix). The slide show below shows some clicker questions we used in our flipped evolution class at CU Boulder.
Using Clickers In Class: Once you have your clicker questions written, then you’ll need to deploy them in class. Below is a basic blueprint for how to run a clicker question
1. Tell students to break into groups and get ready to discuss a clicker question
2. Give students about a minute to discuss the question, and open whatever clicker software you’re using. You’ll usually hear a 30 second surge in talking that dies down after about a minute. After about a minute give students a warning and tell then close out the clicker question.
3. At this point you can show the results of the clicker poll and start to unpack the question. If your questions are challenging, you should be getting significant amounts of wrong answers, so seeing a wide range of answers means you’re doing it right. Usually if 10% of your students are getting the question wrong, it is worth discussing the question in depth
4. Make students be able to articulate why right answers are right and why wrong answers are wrong. You can call on groups to get them to explain their answers (this is nicer than cold-calling individual students). If nobody wants to talk about wrong answers, say something like “why might someone think that B is a tempting answer?” so that nobody has to admit to being wrong in front of their peers.
5. It can be helpful to follow up with another question asking them to apply the material in a different way.
In conclusion, flipping your classroom can be done pretty cheaply and without that much more work than lecturing. This post is really just a starting place, and there are ton of great resources on the web to take you further. I’ve compiled just a few of them below. Good luck and happy flipping!
By Sarah Seiter
Resources
Videos on Flipped Classrooms:https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=EMhJcwvmamY
Resources for Clicker Qs:
Clicker Question Guides from University of Colorado Boulder
http://www.colorado.edu/sei/documents/clickeruse_guide0108.pdf
http://www.slideshare.net/stephaniechasteen/writing-great-clicker-questions
Vanderbilt:
http://cft.vanderbilt.edu/guides-sub-pages/clickers/
Wednesday, May 14, 2014
Addressing the mental health problem in academia
The Guardian UK is publishing an insightful series this May called “Mental health: a university crisis”, as part of Mental Health Month. Although mental health issues for undergraduates are the focus of a variety of different services and programs at most universities, the Guardian includes a unique focus on the issues of academics—graduate students, postdocs, professors and other researchers—for whom it seems that mental health issues are disproportionately common.
The whole series is an important read, and comes at the issue from many different perspectives. A recent survey of university employees not surprisingly found that academics have higher stress levels than other university employees, which they attribute to heavy workloads (!), lack of support (from the department or otherwise), and particularly for early career researchers, feelings of isolation. One particularly insightful piece (with the tagline "I drink too much and haven't had a good night's sleep since last year. Why? Research") argues that academics have particularly unique problems leading to mental health issues. There are typical issues that many high stress jobs include—the ever-regenerating todo list, and the many teaching, research, and service tasks that academics need to accomplish. But academia also seems to attract a high proportion of intense, perfectionistic, passionate people willing to go the extra mile (and encouraged to, given the difficult job market). Worse, research is a creative, even emotional activity – there are highs and lows and periods of intense work that come at the expense of everything else. Ideas are personal, and so the separation between person and research is very slim. The result is often a lack of work-life balance that might produce academic success, but strains mental health. Mental health issues further have dire implications for most research activities, since the symptoms – loss of motivation, concentration and clarity of thought – affect crucial academic skills.
If such issues are so common in academia (and there’s a form of anxiety ubiquitous among graduate students, the imposter syndrome; other common illnesses include anxiety, depression, and panic attacks), why are most of the lecturers and postdocs writing about their mental health experiences for the Guardian choosing to be anonymous? It still seems common to simply downplay or hide problems with stress and mental illness (in the linked study, 61% of academics with mental health problems say their colleagues are unaware of their problems). This may be a reflection of the fact that academia is focused individual performance and individual reputation. Colleagues choose to work with you, to invite you to their department, to hire you, based in no small part on your reputation. Admitting to having suffered from mental illness can feel like adding an obstacle to the already difficult academic landscape. For many, admitting to struggling can feel like failure, particularly since everyone around them seems to be managing the harsh conditions just fine (whether or not that is really true). Academic workdays have less structure than most, which can be isolating. Academics can keep unpredictable hours, disappear for days, send emails at 2 am, sleep at work, and be unkempt and exhausted without much comment; as a result, it can be difficult to identify those colleagues who are at risk (compared to those who are simply unconventional :-) ).
It will be interesting to see where the Guardian series goes. Mental health issues in academia are in many ways the same as those that have affected women and minorities looking for inclusion in academia – subtle comments or stigma, lack of practical support. I remember once hearing a department chair disgusted a co-author who had failed to respond to emails because they were “certifiably crazy; in a mental hospital”. No doubt that was exactly the response the co-author was hoping to avoid. More subtle but more common is lip-service to work-life balance that is counterbalanced by proud references to how hard one or one’s lab works. There is nothing wrong with working hard, but maybe we should temper our praise of sleeping in the lab, coming in every holiday and weekend. It happens and it may be necessary, but is that the badge of honour we really want to claim? It would be sad if the nature of academia, its competitiveness and atmosphere of masochism (“my students are in the lab on Christmas”) limits progress.
Friday, May 9, 2014
Scaling the publication obstacle: the graduate student’s Achilles’ heel
There is no doubt that graduate school can be extremely
stressful and overwhelming. Increasingly, evidence points to these grad school
stressors contributing to mental health problems (articles here
and here).
Many aspects of grad school contribute to self-doubt and unrelenting stress: is
there a job for me after? am I as smart as everyone else? is what I’m doing
even interesting?
But what seems to really exacerbate grad school stress is
the prospect of trying to publish*. The importance of publishing can’t be
dismissed. To be a scientist, you need to publish. There are differing opinions
about what makes a scientist (e.g., is it knowledge, job title,
etc.), but it is clear that if you are not publishing, then you are not
contributing to science. This is what grad students hear, and it is easy to see
how statements like this do not help with the pressure of grad school.
There are other aspects of the grad school experience that
are important, like teaching, taking courses, outreach activities, and serving
on University committees or in leadership positions. These other aspects can be
rewarding because they expand the grad school experience. There is also the sense
that they are under your control and the rewards are more directly influenced
by your efforts. Here then, publishing is different. The publication process does not feel like it is under your control and that the rewards are not necessarily commensurate with
your efforts.
Cartoon by Nick Kim, Massey University, Wellington, accessed here
Given the publishing necessity, how then can grad students
approach it with as little trauma as possible? The publication process will be
experienced differently by different people, some seem like they can shrug off
negative experiences while others internalize them, with negative experiences
gnawing away at their confidence. There is no magic solution to making the
publishing experience better, but here are some suggestions and reassurances.
1) It will never be perfect! I find
myself often telling students to just submit already. There is a tendency to
hold on to a manuscript and read and re-read it. Part of this is the anxiety of
actually submitting it, and procrastination is a result of anxiety. But often
students say that it doesn’t feel ready, or that they are unhappy with part of
the discussion, or that it is not yet perfect. Don’t ever convince yourself
that you will make it perfect –you are setting yourself up for a major
disappointment. Referees ALWAYS criticize, even when they say a paper is good.
There is always room for improvement and you should view the review process as
part of the process that improves papers. If you think of it this way, then
criticisms are less personal (i.e., why didn’t they think it was perfect too?)
and feel more constructive, and you are at peace with submitting something that is less than perfect.
2) Let's dwell on part of the first
point: reviewers ALWAYS criticize. It is part of their job. It is not personal.
Remember, the reviewers are putting time and effort into your paper, and their
comments should be used to make the product better. Reviewers are very honest
and will tell you exactly what could be done to improve a manuscript. They are not attacking you personally, but rather assessing the manuscript.
3) Building on point 2, the
reviewers may not always be correct or provide the best advice. It is OK to
state why you disagree with them. You should always appreciate their efforts
(unless they are unprofessional), but you don’t have to always agree with them.
4) Not every paper is a literature
masterpiece. Effective scientific communication is sometimes best served by
very concise and precise papers. If you have an uncomplicated, relatively
simple experiment, don’t make more complex by writing 20 pages. Notes, Brevia,
Forum papers are all legitimate contributions.
5) Not every paper should be a
Science or Nature paper (or whatever the top journals are in a given
subdiscipline). Confirmatory or localized studies are helpful and necessary.
Large meta-analyses and reviews are not possible without published evidence.
Students should try to think how their work is novel or broadly general (this
is important for selling yourself later on), but it is ok to acknowledge that
your paper is limited in scope or context, and to just send it to the appropriate journal. It takes practice to fit papers to the best journals, so ask colleagues where they would send it. This journal matching can save time and trauma.
6) And here is the important one:
rejection is ok, natural, and normal. We all get rejections. What I mean by
this is that we all get rejections. Your rejection is not abnormal, you don’t
suck more than others, and your experience has been experienced by all the best
scientists. When your paper is reviewed, and then rejected, there is usually
helpful information that should be useful in revising your work to submit
elsewhere. Many journals are inundated with papers and are looking for reasons
to reject. In the journal I edit,
we accept only about 18% of submissions, and so it doesn’t take much to reject
a paper. This is unfortunate, but currently unavoidable (though with the
changing publishing landscape, this norm may change). Rejection is hard, but
don’t take it personally, and feel free to express your rage to your friends.
Publishing is a tricky, but necessary, business for
scientists. When you are having problems with publishing, don’t internalize it.
Instead complain about it to your friends and colleagues. They will undoubtedly
have very similar experiences. Students can be hesitant to share rejections
with other students because they feel inferior, but sharing can be therapeutic.
When I was a postdoc at NCEAS, the
postdocs would share quotes from their worst rejection letters. What would have
normally been a difficult, confidence-bashing experience, became a supportive,
reassuring experience.
Publishing is necessary, but also very stressful and
potentially adding to low-confidence and a feeling that grad school is
overwhelming. I hope that the pointers above can help make the experience less
onerous. But when you do get that acceptance letter telling you that your paper
will be published, hang on to that. Celebrate and know that you have been
rewarded for your hard work, but move on from the rejections.
*I should state that my perspective is from science, and my
views on publishing are very much informed by the publishing culture in
science. I have no way of knowing if the pressures in the humanities or
economics are the same for science students.
Tuesday, April 29, 2014
Unexpected effects of global warming in novel environments: butterflies emerge later in warming urban areas.
There is now ample evidence
that warming temperatures cause advances in the timing of organismal activity
(i.e., phenology). Studies have shown that rising temperatures are responsible
for earlier plant leafing and flowering (Miller-Rushing & Primack 2008,
Wolkovich et al. 2012), pest insect emergence and abundance (Willis et al.
2008), and even local species loss and reduced diversity (Willis et al. 2008).
One emerging expectation from global warming studies is that insects should
emerge earlier since winters are milder and spring temperatures are warmer. This
expectation should hold so long as high temperatures or other environmental
stressors don’t adversely affect the insects. And the concern about shifts in emergence
and insect activity is the potential for mismatches between plant flowering and
the availability of pollinators (Willmer 2012) –if insects emerge too soon,
they may miss the flowers.
Photo by Marc Cadotte
In a forthcoming paper in Ecology by Sarah Diamond and colleagues study 20 common butterfly species across more than 80 sites in Ohio. These sites were located in a range of places across a rural to urban gradient. Instead of finding earlier emergence in warmer places, which were typically urban areas, they found that a number of species were delayed in warmer urban areas. Even though the butterflies might emerge earlier in warmer rural habitats, they were adversely affected in urbanized areas.
These results highlight the need to consider multiple sources of stress from different types of environmental change. Observations from a few locales or from controlled experiments may not lead to conclusions about interactive influences or warming and urbanization, and that's why this study is so important. It observes a counter-intuitive result because of the influence of multiple stressors.
A next step should be to determine if pollinator-plant interactions are being disrupted in these urban areas. The reason why we should care so much about pollinator emergence is that they provide a key ecological service by pollinator wild, garden, and agricultural plants, as well has being an important food source to other species. A mismatch in timing and disrupt these important interactions.
References
Diamond S.E., Cayton H., Wepprich T., Jenkins C.N., Dunn R.R., Haddad
N.M. & Ries L. (2014). Unexpected phenological responses of butterflies to
the interaction of urbanization and geographic temperature. Ecology.
Miller-Rushing A.J. & Primack R.B.
(2008). Global warming and flowering times in Thoreau's Concord: a community
perspective Ecology, 89, 332-341.
Roos J., Hopkins R., Kvarnheden A. &
Dixelius C. (2011). The impact of global warming on plant diseases and insect
vectors in Sweden. Eur J Plant Pathol, 129, 9-19.
Willis C.G., Ruhfel B., Primack R.B.,
Miller-Rushing A.J. & Davis C.C. (2008). Phylogenetic patterns of species
loss in Thoreau's woods are driven by climate change. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 105, 17029-17033.
Willmer P. (2012). Ecology:
pollinator-plant synchrony tested by climate change. Curr. Biol., 22,
R131-R132.
Wolkovich E.M., Cook B.I., Allen J.M.,
Crimmins T.M., Betancourt J.L., Travers S.E., Pau S., Regetz J., Davies T.J.,
Kraft N.J.B., Ault T.R., Bolmgren K., Mazer S.J., McCabe G.J., McGill B.J.,
Parmesan C., Salamin N., Schwartz M.D. & Cleland E.E. (2012). Warming experiments
underpredict plant phenological responses to climate change. Nature,
485, 494-497.
Thursday, April 24, 2014
Data merging: are we moving forward or dealing with Frankenstein's monster
I’m sitting in the Sydney airport waiting for my delayed
flight –which gives me some time to ruminate about the mini-conference I am
leaving. The conference, hosted by the Centre for Biodiversity Analysis (CBA)
and CSIRO
in Australia, on "Understanding biodiversity dynamics using diverse data sources", brought together several fascinating thinkers working on disparate areas including ecology, macroecology, evolution, genomics, and computer science. The goal of the conference was to see if merging different forms of data could lead to greater insights into biodiversity patterns and processes.
Happy integration
On the surface, it seems uncontroversial to say that bringing together different forms of data really does promote new insights into nature. However, this only really works if the data we combine meaningfully complement one another. When researchers bring together data, there are under-appreciated risks, and the resulting effort could result in trying to combine data that make weird bedfellows.
Weird bedfellows
The risks include data that mismatch in the scale of observation, resulting in meaningful variation being missed. Data are often generated according to certain models with specific assumptions, and these data-generation steps can be misunderstood by end-users, resulting in inappropriate uses of data. Further, different data may be combined in standard statistical models, but the linkages between data types is much more subtle and nuanced, requiring alternative models.
Why these are issues stems from the fact that researchers now have an unprecedented access to numerous large data sets. Whether these are large trait data sets, spatial locations, spatial environmental data, genomes, or historical data, they are all built with specific underlying uses, limitations and assumptions.
Regardless of these issues of concern, the opportunity and power to address new questions is greatly enhanced by multiple types of data. One thing I gained from this meeting is that there is a new world of biodiversity analysis and understanding emerging by smart people doing smart things with multiple data. We will soon live in a world where the data and analytical tools allow research to truly combine multiple processes to predict species' distributions, or to move from evolutionary events in deep history to modern day ecological patterns.
Wednesday, April 23, 2014
Guest Post: You teach science, but is your teaching scientific? (Part I)
The first in a series of guest posts about using scientific teaching, active learning, and flipping the classroom by Sarah Seiter, a teaching fellow at the University of Colorado, Boulder.
So how can you use your research skills to boost your teaching? First, you can use teaching techniques that have been empirically tested and rigorously studied, especially a set of techniques called “active learning”. Second, you can collect data on yourself and your students to gauge your progress and adjust your teaching as needed, a process called “formative assessment”. While this can seem daunting, it helps to remember that as a researcher you’re uniquely equipped to overhaul your teaching, using the skills you already rely on in the lab and the field. Like a lot of paradigm shifts in science, using data to guide your teaching seems pretty obvious after the fact, but it can be revolutionary for you and your students.
What is Active Learning:
There are a lot of definitions of active learning floating around, but in short active learning techniques force students to engage with the material, while it is being taught. More importantly, students practice the material and make mistakes while they are surrounded by a community of peers and instructors who can help. There are a lot of ways to bring active learning strategies to your classroom, such as clicker response systems (handheld devices that allow them to take short quizzes throughout the lecture). Case studies are another tool: students read about scientific problems and then apply the information to real world problems (medical and law schools have been them for years). I’ll get into some more examples of these techniques in post II; there are lots of free and awesome resources that will allow you to try active learning techniques in your class with minimal investment.Formative Assessment:
The other way data can help you overhaul your class is through formative assessment, a series of small, frequent, low stakes assessment of student learning. A lot of college courses use what’s called summative assessment – one or two major exams that test a semester’s worth of material, with a few labs or a term paper for balance. If your goal is to see if your students learned anything over a semester this is probably sufficient. This is also fine if you’re trying to weed out underperforming students from your major (but seriously, don’t do that). But if you’re interested in coaching students towards mastery of the subject matter, it probably isn’t enough to just tell them how much they learned after half the class is over. If you think about learning goals like we think of fitness goals, this is like asking students to qualify for the Boston marathon, without giving them any times for their training runs.Formative assessment can be done in many ways: weekly quizzes or taking data with classroom clicker systems. While a lot of formative assessment research focuses on measuring student progress, instructors have lots to gain by measuring their own pedagogical skills. There are a lot of tools out there to measure improvement in teaching skills (K-12 teachers have been getting formatively assessed for years), but even setting simple goals for yourself (“make at least 5 minutes for student questions”) and monitoring your progress can be really helpful. Post III will talk about how to do (relatively) painless formative assessment in your class.
How does this work and who does it work for:
Scientific teaching is revolutionary because it works for everyone, faculty and students alike. However, it has particularly useful benefits for some types of instructors and students.New Faculty: inexperienced faculty can achieve results as good or better than experienced faculty by using evidence based teaching techniques. In a study at the University of Colorado, physics students taught by a graduate TA using scientific teaching outperformed those taught by an experienced (and well loved) professor using a standard lecture style (you can read the study here). Faculty who are not native English speakers, or who are simply shy can get a lot of leverage using scientific teaching techniques, because doing in-class activities relieves the pressure to deliver perfect lectures.
Test scores between a lecture-taught physics section and a section taught using active learning techniques. |
Seasoned Faculty: For faculty who already have their teaching style established, scientific teaching can spice up lectures that have become rote or help you address concepts that you see students struggle with year after year. Even if you feel like you have your lectures completely dialed in, consider whether you’re using the most cutting edge techniques in your lab, and if you your classroom deserves the same treatment.
Students also stand to gain from scientific teaching, and some groups of students are particularly poised to benefit from it:
Students who don’t plan to go into science: Even in majors classes, most of the students we teach won’t go on to become scientists. But skills like analyzing data, and writing convincing evidence based arguments are useful in almost any field. Active learning trains students to be smart consumers of information, and formative assessment teaches students to monitor their own learning – two skills we could stand to see more of in any career.
Students Who Love Science: Active learning can give star students a leg up on the skills they’ll need to succeed as academics, for all the reasons listed above. Occasionally really bright students will balk at active learning, because having to wrestle with complicated data makes them feel stupid. While it can feel awful to watch your smartest students struggle, it is important to remember that real scientists have to confront confusing data every day. For students who want research careers, learning to persevere through messy and inconclusive results is critical.
Students who struggle with science: Active learning can be a great leveler for students who come from disadvantaged backgrounds. A University of Washington study showed that active learning and student peer tutoring could eliminate achievement gaps for minority students. If you partially got into academia because you wanted to make a difference in educating young people, here is one empirically proven way to do that.
Are there downsides?
Like anything, active learning involves tradeoffs. While the overwhelming evidence suggests that active learning is the best way to train new faculty (the white house even published a report calling for more of it!), there are sometimes roadblocks to scientific teaching.Content Isn’t King Anymore: Taking time to work with data, or apply scientific research to policy problems takes more time, so instructors can cover fewer examples in class. In active learning, students are developing scientific skills like experimental design or technical writing, but after spending an hour hammering out an experiment to test the evolution of virulence, they often feel like they’ve only learned about “one stupid disease”. However, there is lots of evidence that covering topics in depth is more beneficial than doing a survey of many topics. For example, high schoolers that studied a single subject in depth for more than a month were more likely to declare a science major in college than students who covered more topics.
Demands on Instructor Time: I actually haven’t found that active learning takes more time to prepare –case studies and clickers actually take a up a decent amount of class time, so I spend less time prepping and rehearsing lectures. However, if you already have a slide deck you’ve been using for years, developing clicker questions and class exercises requires an upfront investment of time. Formative assessment can also take more time, although online quiz tools and peer grading can help take some of the pressure off instructors.
If you want to learn more about the theory behind scientific teaching there are a lot of great resources on the subject:
These podcasts are a great place to start:
http://americanradioworks.publicradio.org/features/tomorrows-college/lectures/
http://www.slate.com/articles/podcasts/education/2013/12/schooled_podcast_the_flipped_classroom.html
This book is a classic in the field:
http://www.amazon.com/Scientific-Teaching-Jo-Handelsman/dp/1429201886
By Sarah Seiter
Monday, April 21, 2014
Null models matter, but what should they look like?
Neutral Biogeography and the Evolution of Climatic Niches. Florian C. Boucher, Wilfried Thuiller, T. Jonathan Davies, and Sébastien Lavergne. The American Naturalist, Vol. 183, No. 5 (May 2014), pp. 573-584
Null models have become a fundamental part of community ecology. For the most part, this is an improvement over our null-model free days: patterns are now interpreted with reference to patterns that might arise through chance and in the absence of ecological processes of interest. Null models today are ubiquitous in tests of phylogenetic signals, patterns of species co-occurrence, models of species distribution-climate relationships. But even though null models are a success in that they are widespread and commonly used, there are problems--in particular, there is a disconnect between how null models are chosen and interpreted and what information they actually provide. Unfortunately, simple and easily applied null models tend to be favoured, but they are often interpreted as though they are complicated, mechanism-explicit models.
The new paper “Neutral Biogeography and the Evolution of Climatic Niches” from Boucher et al. provides a good example of this problem. The premise of the paper is straightforward: studies of phylogenetic niche conservation tend to rely on simple null models, and as a result may misinterpret what their data shows because of the type of null models that they use. The study of phylogenetic niche conservation and niche evolution is becoming increasingly popular, particularly studies on how species' climatic niches evolve and how climate niches relate to patterns of diversity. In a time of changing climates, there are also important applications looking at how species respond to climatic shifts. Studies of changes in climate niches through evolutionary time usually rely on a definition of the climate niche based on empirical data, more specifically, the mean position of a given species along a continuous abiotic gradient. Because this is not directly tied to physiological measurements, climate niche data may also capture the effect of dispersal limitations or biotic interactions. Hence the need for null models, however the null models used in these studies primarily flag changes in climate niche that result from to random drift or selection in a varying environment. These types of null models use Brownian motion (a "random walk") to answer questions about whether niches are more or less similar than expected due to chance, or else whether a particular model of niche evolution is a better fit to the data than a model of Brownian motion.
The authors suggest that the reliance on Brownian motion is problematic, since these simple null models cannot actually distinguish between patterns of climate niches that arise simply due to speciation and migration but no selection on climate niches, and those that are the result of true niche evolution. If this is true, conclusions about niche evolution may be suspect, since they depend on the null model used. The authors used a neutral, spatially explicit model (known as an "alternative neutral biogeographic model") that simulates dynamics driven only by speciation and migration, with species being neutral in their dynamics. This provides an alternative model of patterns that may arise in climate niches among species, despite the absence of direct selection on the trait. The paper then looks at whether climatic niches exhibit phylogenetic signals when they arise via neutral spatial dynamics; if gradualism a reasonable neutral expectation for the evolution of climatic niches on geological timescales; and whether constraints on climatic niche diversification can arise simply through bounded geographic space. Simulations of the neutral biogeographic model used a gridded “continent” with variable climate conditions: each cell has a carrying capacity, and species move via migration and split into two species either by point mutation, or else by vicariance (a geographic barrier appears, leading to divergence of 2 populations). Not surprisingly, their results show that even in the absence of any selection on species’ climate niches, patterns can result that differ greatly from a simple Brownian motion-based null model. So the simple null model (Brownian motion) often concluded that results from the more complex null model were different from the random/null expectation. This isn't a problem per se. The problem is that currently interpretations of the Brownian motion model may be that anything different from null is a signal for niche evolution (or conservation). Obviously that is not correct.
This paper is focused on the issue of choosing null models for studies of climate niche evolution, but it fits into a current of thought about the problems with how ecologists are using null models. It is one thing to know that you need and want to use a null model, but it is much more difficult to construct an appropriate null model, and interpret the output correctly. Null models (such as the Brownian motion null model) are often so simplistic that they are straw man arguments – if ecology isn't the result of only randomness, your null model is pretty likely to be a poor fit to the data. On the other hand, the more specific and complex the null model is, the easier it is to throw the baby out with the bathwater. Given how much data is interpreted in the light of null models, it seems that choosing and interpreting null models needs to be more of a priority.
Null models have become a fundamental part of community ecology. For the most part, this is an improvement over our null-model free days: patterns are now interpreted with reference to patterns that might arise through chance and in the absence of ecological processes of interest. Null models today are ubiquitous in tests of phylogenetic signals, patterns of species co-occurrence, models of species distribution-climate relationships. But even though null models are a success in that they are widespread and commonly used, there are problems--in particular, there is a disconnect between how null models are chosen and interpreted and what information they actually provide. Unfortunately, simple and easily applied null models tend to be favoured, but they are often interpreted as though they are complicated, mechanism-explicit models.
The new paper “Neutral Biogeography and the Evolution of Climatic Niches” from Boucher et al. provides a good example of this problem. The premise of the paper is straightforward: studies of phylogenetic niche conservation tend to rely on simple null models, and as a result may misinterpret what their data shows because of the type of null models that they use. The study of phylogenetic niche conservation and niche evolution is becoming increasingly popular, particularly studies on how species' climatic niches evolve and how climate niches relate to patterns of diversity. In a time of changing climates, there are also important applications looking at how species respond to climatic shifts. Studies of changes in climate niches through evolutionary time usually rely on a definition of the climate niche based on empirical data, more specifically, the mean position of a given species along a continuous abiotic gradient. Because this is not directly tied to physiological measurements, climate niche data may also capture the effect of dispersal limitations or biotic interactions. Hence the need for null models, however the null models used in these studies primarily flag changes in climate niche that result from to random drift or selection in a varying environment. These types of null models use Brownian motion (a "random walk") to answer questions about whether niches are more or less similar than expected due to chance, or else whether a particular model of niche evolution is a better fit to the data than a model of Brownian motion.
The authors suggest that the reliance on Brownian motion is problematic, since these simple null models cannot actually distinguish between patterns of climate niches that arise simply due to speciation and migration but no selection on climate niches, and those that are the result of true niche evolution. If this is true, conclusions about niche evolution may be suspect, since they depend on the null model used. The authors used a neutral, spatially explicit model (known as an "alternative neutral biogeographic model") that simulates dynamics driven only by speciation and migration, with species being neutral in their dynamics. This provides an alternative model of patterns that may arise in climate niches among species, despite the absence of direct selection on the trait. The paper then looks at whether climatic niches exhibit phylogenetic signals when they arise via neutral spatial dynamics; if gradualism a reasonable neutral expectation for the evolution of climatic niches on geological timescales; and whether constraints on climatic niche diversification can arise simply through bounded geographic space. Simulations of the neutral biogeographic model used a gridded “continent” with variable climate conditions: each cell has a carrying capacity, and species move via migration and split into two species either by point mutation, or else by vicariance (a geographic barrier appears, leading to divergence of 2 populations). Not surprisingly, their results show that even in the absence of any selection on species’ climate niches, patterns can result that differ greatly from a simple Brownian motion-based null model. So the simple null model (Brownian motion) often concluded that results from the more complex null model were different from the random/null expectation. This isn't a problem per se. The problem is that currently interpretations of the Brownian motion model may be that anything different from null is a signal for niche evolution (or conservation). Obviously that is not correct.
This paper is focused on the issue of choosing null models for studies of climate niche evolution, but it fits into a current of thought about the problems with how ecologists are using null models. It is one thing to know that you need and want to use a null model, but it is much more difficult to construct an appropriate null model, and interpret the output correctly. Null models (such as the Brownian motion null model) are often so simplistic that they are straw man arguments – if ecology isn't the result of only randomness, your null model is pretty likely to be a poor fit to the data. On the other hand, the more specific and complex the null model is, the easier it is to throw the baby out with the bathwater. Given how much data is interpreted in the light of null models, it seems that choosing and interpreting null models needs to be more of a priority.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)